Explanatory Feedback: Difference between revisions

From ECT wiki
 
(3 intermediate revisions by one other user not shown)
Line 5: Line 5:
=='''Evidence'''==
=='''Evidence'''==


Roxana Moreno[https://www.wikidata.org/wiki/Q78698899] (2004) recruited 49 college students to conduct an experiment demonstrating explanatory feedback's favorable impact on learning. He randomly assigned these students to two groups, one group provided explanatory feedback (EF group), and the other group provided corrective feedback (CF group). All the students took the prior knowledge test to ensure they were novices, and they will learn how to design plants through a multimedia learning program later. During the learning process, students in the EF group will receive verbal explanations from a software pedagogic agent, informing students whether their choices are correct and explaining why they are correct or wrong (Moreno, 2004). The students in the CF group will only be informed whether their options are correct by the software pedagogic agent(Moreno, 2004). Afterward, Roxana conducted a multivariate analysis on these students to compare the differences in their learning outcomes. Results show that when the agent provides explanatory feedback alongside corrective feedback during the knowledge construction process, students have higher retention and transfer levels and more positive ratings of the difficulty and usefulness of the instructional program than students who have corrective feedback alone (Moreno, 2004).
Roxana Moreno[https://www.wikidata.org/wiki/Q78698899] (2004) recruited 49 college students to conduct an experiment demonstrating explanatory feedback's favorable impact on learning. He randomly assigned these students to two groups, one group provided explanatory feedback (EF group), and the other group provided corrective feedback (CF group). All the students took the prior knowledge test to ensure they were novices, and they will learn how to design plants through a multimedia learning program later. During the learning process, students in the EF group will receive verbal explanations from a software pedagogic agent, informing students whether their choices are correct and explaining why they are correct or wrong (Moreno, 2004). The students in the CF group will only be informed whether their options are correct by the software pedagogic agent(Moreno, 2004). Afterward, Roxana conducted a multivariate analysis on these students to compare the differences in their learning outcomes. Results show that when the agent provides explanatory feedback alongside corrective feedback during the knowledge construction process, students have higher retention and [[transfer]] levels and more positive ratings of the difficulty and usefulness of the instructional program than students who have corrective feedback alone (Moreno, 2004).
[[File:Explanatory Feedback Example.png|thumb|Explanatory Feedback Example]]
[[File:Explanatory Feedback Example.png|thumb|Explanatory Feedback Example]]
[[File:Example of Corrective Feedback.jpg|thumb|Corrective Feedback Example]]
[[File:Example of Corrective Feedback.jpg|thumb|Corrective Feedback Example]]

Latest revision as of 22:38, 24 December 2022

Overview[edit | edit source]

Explanatory feedback provides learners with an explanation of whether their answers are correct, which can help learners avoid misunderstanding and help establish a correct mental model (Clark & Mayer, 2011) to achieve more effective learning outcomes. It is an essential concept in the feedback principle proposed by Richard E. Mayer[1] and Logan Fiorella. They believe that novice students learn better when they have explanatory feedback than when they are given corrective feedback[2] alone (Mayer & Fiorella, 2021).

Evidence[edit | edit source]

Roxana Moreno[3] (2004) recruited 49 college students to conduct an experiment demonstrating explanatory feedback's favorable impact on learning. He randomly assigned these students to two groups, one group provided explanatory feedback (EF group), and the other group provided corrective feedback (CF group). All the students took the prior knowledge test to ensure they were novices, and they will learn how to design plants through a multimedia learning program later. During the learning process, students in the EF group will receive verbal explanations from a software pedagogic agent, informing students whether their choices are correct and explaining why they are correct or wrong (Moreno, 2004). The students in the CF group will only be informed whether their options are correct by the software pedagogic agent(Moreno, 2004). Afterward, Roxana conducted a multivariate analysis on these students to compare the differences in their learning outcomes. Results show that when the agent provides explanatory feedback alongside corrective feedback during the knowledge construction process, students have higher retention and transfer levels and more positive ratings of the difficulty and usefulness of the instructional program than students who have corrective feedback alone (Moreno, 2004).

Explanatory Feedback Example
Corrective Feedback Example

Design Implications[edit | edit source]

We can often see the role of explanatory feedback in online school exams. For example, in a calculus exam, the grader only pointed out the wrong answer without providing any explanation, which is a form of corrective feedback. This grading method made all students very confused and didn't know how to understand the questions they did wrong, and it also prevented them from learning from their mistakes. After students reported this problem to the grader, in addition to pointing out students' mistakes in the subsequent exams, the grader will also add his comments next to his marks, which is a form of explanatory feedback. After changing his grading strategy, all students who took the exam can self-reflect when faced with their own mistakes and think about the correct problem-solving method according to the grader's explanation. It also greatly improves the effectiveness and efficiency of students' learning. Students no longer need to spend extra time searching for relevant information, and they can also avoid going on the wrong path due to their misunderstanding of concepts.

Related: Cognitive Load Theory[edit | edit source]

Richard E. Mayer and Logan Fiorella also mentioned in their article that the delivery method of feedback may bring extraneous cognitive load to learners, thus affecting explanatory feedback's effectiveness (Clark & Mayer, 2011). The extraneous cognitive load refers to the cognitive load brought to students due to inappropriate instructional procedures (Sweller, 2010). According to Cognitive Load Theory[4], learners' working memory capacity is limited, and cognitive load refers to the number of working memory resources used(Sweller, 2010). When learners' working memory is overloaded, students will not be able to process new knowledge, and explanatory feedback will become less effective.

References[edit | edit source]


Return to [Main Page] | [Top Page]